What makes time fly




















Van Wassenhove and colleagues conducted this experiment during functional magnetic resonance imaging, which enabled them to examine how the brain reacted differently to looming and receding.

The brain imaging data revealed two main findings. First, structures in the middle of the brain were more active during the looming condition. These brain areas are also known to activate in experiments that involve the comparison of self-judgments to the judgments of others, or when an experimenter does not tell the subject what to do. In both cases, the prevailing idea is that the brain is busy wondering about itself, its ongoing plans and activities, and relating oneself to the rest of the world.

Second, brain areas including the left anterior insula were more active during the receding condition relative to the looming condition. The insula as a whole has been the focus of many recent studies and is thought to be involved in complex emotional processing.

In particular, Craig has suggested that there is an emotional asymmetry , in which the left forebrain is associated with approach, safety, positive affect and the right forebrain is associated with arousal, danger, and negative affect.

An object moving away might be seen as non-threatening, signaling the self to relax. In fact, some investigators have suggested that the amount of energy spent during thinking and experiencing defines the subjective experience of duration.

In other words, the more energy it takes to process a stimulus the longer it appears as a subjective experience of time. Something moving toward you has more relevance than the same stimulus moving away from you: You may need to prepare somehow; time seems to move more slowly.

The experience of time is not linear. Fear and joy stretch time as do stimuli that move towards us. What can we learn from these studies for our day-to-day experiences?

When we are children, time ambles by, with endless car journeys and summer holidays which seem to last forever. But as adults, time seems to speed up at a frightening rate, with Christmas and birthdays arriving more quickly every year. Our experience of time is flexible, speeding up in some situations and slowing down in others. So maybe by understanding the psychological processes behind our different experiences of time, we might be able to slow things down a little. One of these is that time seems to speed up as we get older.

These two laws are caused by the same underlying factor: the relationship between our experience of time and the amount of information including perceptions, sensations and thoughts our minds process. The more information our minds take in, the slower time seems to pass. This partly explains why time passes so slowly for children and seems to speed up as we get older.

For children, the world is a fascinating place, full of new experiences and fresh sensations. As we get older, we have fewer new experiences and the world around us becomes more and more familiar. It's no fun when time seems to drag on, and you might be left wondering if it's ever possible to make the time pass more quickly. Here's an overview of how time perception works and some strategies to help you make the time go by faster.

Although standard measurements of time like minutes, hours, and days exist, the way you perceive these temporal units can change, depending on what you're doing and how you're feeling. Throughout a typical day or week, the way we perceive time usually roughly matches up with the objective time of clocks and calendars.

But when routine is broken in some way—whether it's a novel experience, an intense emotion, or lack of activity—how we perceive the passage of time is affected. Michael Flaherty, Ph. The way we experience time depends on what he calls " the density of human experience ," which measures the volume of both objective and subjective information.

The density of human experience is high when a lot is happening. But paradoxically, the density of experience is also high when seemingly nothing is going on. This "empty" period of time is actually filled with subjective experience.

You're turning inward and concentrating on your own actions and surroundings. When you experience an intense psychological reaction, whether it's boredom, shock, fear, or anxiety, the density of human experience is high, and time seems to pass more slowly.

Negative emotions, like stress, can cause our perception of time to slow down. In some cases, time slowing down in stressful situations can actually be useful, such as in a dangerous moment when you have to act quickly.

However, the situation is much different when you're sitting at home anxiously pondering your future or waiting for someone to text you back. Since perception of time can be so subjective, different strategies will work for different people. But overall, it's about getting into a routine or a flow where the density of experience is low and your negative emotions are reduced. Here are some activities that can help:. According to research published in the Journal of Experimental Psychology, time is perceived as going by much faster when people are engaged in a routine.

Density of experience is low in a routine situation since it doesn't require extra attention. Creating a daily routine in which you can get into a state of flow can help reduce boredom and make the time go by faster. When you're busy, you're less likely to notice the passage of time as much as when you have nothing else to focus on. We all know that time flies when you're having fun—but it's the type of fun that really matters. According to a study published in Psychological Science , you should focus on "goal-motivated" activities that you actually enjoy.

Since negative emotions cause time to crawl, engage in self-care habits that help eliminate stress and anxiety.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000